Skip to main content

Look out! The Backyard Bird Alarm Call Network

By Gustave Axelson
From the Winter 2016 issue of Living Bird magazine.
Black-capped Chickadee by Steve Gettle/Minden Pictures.
Erick Greene and his team employ “roboraptors”—taxidermied hawks and owls with robotic moving parts—to learn about how birds communicate with alarm calls when a predator is nearby. Video by Chris Foito/Cornell Lab Multimedia.

The woods are wiretapped. Everywhere eyes are watching. And the moment a Black-capped Chickadee sees something to be concerned about—say the slim silhouette of a Sharp-shinned Hawk perched in a tree—a rapid-response network kicks into action.

Chick-a-dee-dee-dee! The alarm call ricochets from chickadees to nuthatch­es to titmice to jays, and soon an angry horde of songbirds arrives to mob the intruder. Even after the sharpie gives up and flies off, the alarm call network keeps buzzing with a wave of real-time reports that the threat is now on the move. This new information can trav­el at speeds exceeding 100 miles per hour, giving birds advance warning to take cover.

After 10 years of studying alarm calls, University of Montana research­er Erick Greene says it’s a wonder predators ever beat the system.

“It’s really hard to be a hawk,” Greene says. “You’re hunting and fly­ing around, trying to find your dinner, and yet there’s this bow wave of infor­mation that travels out in front of you faster than you can ever fly.”

Greene was part of a team that made newspaper headlines in 2005 with a paper in the journal Science that decoded the chick-a-dee alarm call of Black-capped Chickadees. They found that the number of -dees in a call cor­related with the threat posed by a pred­ator. More -dees mean a higher threat. So a Northern Pygmy-Owl (which specializes in hunting small birds) elic­ited a string of –dees from chickadees, sometimes as many as 12.

Now Greene is teaming up with Chris Clark of the Cornell Lab of Or­nithology’s Bioacoustics Research Program and Mike Webster of the Macaulay Library on a National Sci­ence Foundation-funded project to delve even deeper into the complex communications that are alarm calls. Greene and his team are finding that different alarm calls can have specific meanings—that alarm calls are actu­ally a spoken language among birds, and several species understand what’s being said.

In some experiments, Greene and his team play audio recordings of dif­ferent species’ alarm calls near bird feeders and observe how the birds react. In others, the researchers em­ploy roboraptors—taxidermied hawks and owls with robotic moving parts. On a sunny spring day last May at the Cornell Lab’s Sapsucker Woods head­quarters in Upstate New York, Greene pressed the button on a modified ga­rage door opener to drop a cloth cur­tain and reveal an animatronic Eastern Screech-Owl. Left to right, the robo­raptor slowly swiveled its head (not un­like the mechanical children at Disney World who sing “It’s a Small World”). A pair of Tufted Titmice descended down the boughs of a nearby tree immedi­ately, scolding all the way. Chickadees soon joined the protest to take turns dive-bombing the owl until the curtain went back up and the show was over. All the while, an array of microphones hidden in the underbrush captured ev­ery uttered sound, adding to Greene’s storehouse of alarm-call data.

Back on his home turf near Mis­soula, Montana, Greene and his team are analyzing the spectrograms of thousands of alarm calls in audio labs. When Greene and graduate student Alexis Billings took a closer look at the harsh, guttural wah-wah-wah mob­bing calls of Steller’s Jays—meant to intimidate a robotic Northern Gos­hawk—they noticed that these were loud, harsh, large-bandwidth sounds. In other words, sounds designed to travel far and wide and be very easy to locate. Greene says these kinds of calls are given for a stationary threat, such as a perched raptor.

More than 50 different species will respond to a chickadee’s alarm call. The woods are wired by these alarm call networks, which allow various species of birds and small mammals to exchange information about a hawk or an owl. In the East and the West, the specific members of the alarm call network may differ but the acoustic properties of how alarm calls are transmitted are similar. Photos (from top to bottom): White-breasted Nuthatch by Kirchmeier, Blue Jay by Larry Eiden, American Red Squirrel by Guy Lichter, Tufted Timouse by Deborah E. Bifulco.
More than 50 different species will respond to a chickadee’s alarm call. The woods are wired by these alarm call networks, which allow various species of birds and small mammals to exchange information about a hawk or an owl. In the East and the West, the specific members of the alarm call network may differ but the acoustic properties of how alarm calls are transmitted are similar. Photos (from top to bottom): White-breasted Nuthatch by Kirchmeier, Blue Jay by Larry Eiden, American Red Squirrel by Guy Lichter, Tufted Timouse by Deborah E. Bifulco.

“The intent here is to call in the troops,” Greene says. “It may seem counterintuitive for birds to mobilize and fly toward a threat, but a perched raptor is not as dangerous as a flying, actively hunting raptor. The idea is to gather as many jays as possible and make a bunch of loud, raucous calls to let this hawk know its cover is blown and it has little chance of hunting suc­cessfully, so it might as well move on.”

On the other hand, Greene has re­corded alarm calls that sound like seet-seet-seet and have a different meaning. Seets are high-pitched calls given when a raptor is in flight. For the raptor, it’s hard to tell where these sounds are coming from. But small birds hear it and know immediately what to do—duck into cover or freeze. In one ex­periment in Montana, a peripatetic Red-breasted Nuthatch was observed bouncing around feeder to feeder, but then Greene broadcast a snippet of seet calls lasting a single second. Instantly the nuthatch froze in place, seed in its bill, and stayed that way—an ornamen­tal bird statue—for half an hour.

“Alarm calls evoke a powerful response,” Greene says. “For birds, it’s life or death.”

Because seet calls are higher frequency than mobbing calls, they don’t transmit as far. And yet, the message still moves quickly throughout the woods, thanks to a rapid-relay system. Think of the old-time bucket brigades used to put out a fire. In experiments using extensive networks of microphones placed every 50 meters—covering the acoustic landscape, as Greene says—he has recorded seet calls re­layed across several different species, from Black-capped Chickadees to Lazuli Buntings to House Finches to Song Sparrows. The microphone array clocked the alarm signal traveling up to 100 mph from an initial point to 200 meters away (about the length of two football fields).

More than 50 different species are known to respond to a chickadee’s mobbing calls. Alarm calls can be understood easily by different species because, even though each species has its own call, the acoustic proper­ties are similar. All mobbing calls are large bandwidth. (Birders, you’re ac­tually mimicking a mobbing call every time you engage in pishing, which is a large-bandwidth sound.)

All seet calls are in the same frequency range. Other research has shown that Australian fairywrens will flee in response to the seet calls of species they’ve never heard before. Greene has documented that even red squirrels and chipmunks can understand the meaning of seet calls, and vice versa—the alarm squeaks of red squirrels elicit a take-cover response among chickadees.

“It’s the universal language of ‘look out!’” says Janelle Morano, a Cornell Lab researcher who’s coordinating the experiments in Ithaca, New York. Morano says she was amazed the first time she lowered the curtain to reveal a robotic owl at a backyard bird feeder.

“Everybody stopped feeding. Then the chickadees and titmice went crazy. Then before you know it, there’s Blue Jays squawking, squirrels screeching, it’s a madhouse.”

She was doubly amazed when she got back into the Macaulay Library studio and downloaded the record­ings for analysis. The chorus of alarm calls had ballooned from a few birds to more than 20 individuals. In just 5 minutes, she had recorded more than 800 chickadee call notes alone.

“There’s so much data to be ana­lyzed every time an alarm call goes out,” she says.

The Cornell Lab

All About Birds
is a free resource

Available for everyone,
funded by donors like you

American Kestrel by Blair Dudeck / Macaulay Library

Related Stories